GIBSON J.J.1977.THE THEORY OF AFFORDANCES PDF

Relevant Developments During the s and s, an increasing number of scholars engaged in the development of the ecological approach. Previous theories of human movement e. An outline of a theory of affordances. Login using His doctoral research focused on memory and learning, and constituted the basis of his first publication Gibson Besides, this body schema of actions and signs gihson a network of attractions and repulsions similar to the way in which J. Experimenters found that participants accurately matched the length of the rods.

Author:Nera Dairamar
Country:Barbados
Language:English (Spanish)
Genre:Literature
Published (Last):13 August 2016
Pages:119
PDF File Size:13.60 Mb
ePub File Size:5.32 Mb
ISBN:627-6-29883-320-1
Downloads:11840
Price:Free* [*Free Regsitration Required]
Uploader:Toktilar



Affordances and Design 17 November In the world of design, the term "affordance" has taken on a life far beyond the original meaning. It might help if we return to the original definition. Let me try to clarify the definition of the term and its many uses. Gibson , to refer to the actionable properties between the world and an actor a person or animal. To Gibson, affordances are a relationship. They are a part of nature: they do not have to be visible, known, or desirable.

Some affordances are yet to be discovered. Some are dangerous. I suspect that none of us know all the affordances of even everyday objects. The concept has caught on, but not always with true understanding. Part of the blame lies with me: I should have used the term "perceived affordance," for in design, we care much more about what the user perceives than what is actually true. What the designer cares about is whether the user perceives that some action is possible or in the case of perceived non-affordances, not possible.

In product design, where one deals with real, physical objects, there can be both real and perceived affordances, and the two need not be the same.

In graphical, screen-based interfaces, all that the designer has available is control over perceived affordances. The computer system, with its keyboard, display screen, pointing device e.

Most of this affordance is of no value. Thus, if the display does not have a touch-sensitive screen, the screen still affords touching, but it has no result on the computer system. Mind you, the affordance still has impact: it is useful in multiple-person communication, and it helps aid the sale of screen-cleaning tissues and fluids. All screens afford touching: only some detect the touch and are capable of responding. But the affordance of touchability is the same in all cases.

Touch sensitive screens often make their affordance visibly perceivable by displaying a cursor under the pointing spot. The cursor is not an affordance; it is visual feedback. In similar vein, because I can click anytime I want, it is wrong to argue whether a graphical object on the screen "affords clicking.

The real question is about the perceived affordance: Does the user perceive that clicking on that location is a meaningful, useful action to perform? Physical constraints are closely related to real affordances: Thus, it is not possible to move the cursor outside the screen: this is a physical constraint.

Logical constraints use reasoning to determine the alternatives. Thus, if we ask the user to click on 5 locations and only 4 are immediately visible; the person knows, logically, that there is still location left. Cultural constraints are learned conventions that are shared by a cultural group. The fact that the graphic on the right hand side of a display is a "scroll bar" and that one should move the cursor to it, hold down a mouse button, and "drag" it downward in order to see objects located below the current visible set thus causing the image itself to appear to move upwards -- all this is a cultural, learned convention.

The choice of action is arbitrary: there is nothing inherent in the devices or design that requires the system to act in this way. The word "arbitrary" does not mean that any random depiction would do equally well: the current choice is an intelligent fit to human cognition, but this is a different topic, for another day.

In all scrolling bars I have seen, there is a physical constraint as well: the vertical scroll bar only moves vertically, not horizontally. Once again, these cultural constraints are arbitrary. The scroll bar could have been designed very differently. It could have been located on the left, or the top, or as a button.

Today, its action follows the "outside-in" convention, but in the early days, a number of designs used the "inside-out" convention, so that moving the scroll bar down caused the text to move downwards, exposing the new text at the top of the screen. Today if we were to use that convention, it would be confusing, but only because the convention has already been established. Some drawing programs still follow that earlier convention and they have adopted the convention of a "hand" graphic to signal that moving the cursor upwards "grabs the text" and moves it upward as well.

All this is cultural convention, not affordances. In the world of design, what matters is: If the desired controls can be perceived1. In an easy to use design, if they can both readily be perceived and interpreted If the desired actions can be discovered2. Whether standard conventions are obeyed In a game, the designer may deliberately violate the A principles 1A and 2A. Sometimes, even in effective, mass-consumer designs, it is useful to violate these principles.

Thus, sometimes there are "back doors" for those who maintain the system, and it is not desirable that the average user know of their existence. But if the user is to be effective at, these principles should be followed. How new users understand what to do: Four principles for screen interfaces There are several ways of getting a new user to understand what actions are possible. This was a major theme of POET, but there, the emphasis was on physical products, so perceived physical affordances were extremely important.

In graphical design e. Here are four, but like most design conventions, each has both virtues and drawbacks: 1. Follow conventional usage, both in the choice of images and the allowable interactions. Convention severely constrains creativity.

Following convention may also violate intellectual property laws. Sometimes we wish to introduce a new kind of action for which there are, as yet, no accepted conventions.

On the whole, however, unless we follow the major conventions, we are doomed to fail. Those who violate conventions, even when they are convinced that their new method is superior, are doomed to fail.

You cannot successfully introduce a non-qwerty keyboard today, or reverse the window scroll bar convention, or suddenly require double-clicking on web links. For better or for worse, human culture changes slowly, if at all. Use words to describe the desired action e. This is, of course, why menus can be relatively easy to understand: the resulting action is described verbally. Of course, the method of using the menu has to be learned, and the text still has to be chosen with care -- and user tested.

Words alone cannot solve the problem, for there still must be some way of knowing what action and where it is to be done. This requires a convention of highlighting, or outlining, or depiction of a actionable object. It is also well known that single word labels fail for most people -- everyone has a favorite word, but the variety of preferred words is overwhelming. Words also cause problems with international adoption. Thus, road signs often use graphics -- an international standard on road sign graphics exists.

Alas, most people do not understand those standards. It is also the case that words are understood more quickly than graphics -- even a well known, understood graphic. Words plus graphics are even more readily understood. Use metaphor. Metaphor is both useful and harmful. I personally believe that metaphors are more harmful than useful, but this is a different topic for a different day. Worse, they may take the metaphor too literally and try to do actions that were not intended.

Still, this is one way of training users. Follow a coherent conceptual model so that once part of the interface is learned, the same principles apply to other parts.

Coherent conceptual models are valuable and, in my opinion, necessary, but there still remains the bootstrapping problem; how does one learn the model in the first place? References The primary reference to affordances in perception is Gibson, , but also see Gibson, Gibson, J. The theory of affordances. Bransford Eds.

The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Norman, D. The psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books. The design of everyday things:Revised and expanded edition. New York: Doubleday. Essay types.

EPITEZAN BULA PDF

James J. Gibson

Affordances and Design 17 November In the world of design, the term "affordance" has taken on a life far beyond the original meaning. It might help if we return to the original definition. Let me try to clarify the definition of the term and its many uses. Gibson , to refer to the actionable properties between the world and an actor a person or animal. To Gibson, affordances are a relationship. They are a part of nature: they do not have to be visible, known, or desirable. Some affordances are yet to be discovered.

MANDIRI INVESTA ATRAKTIF PDF

Affordances and Design

Gibson recalled being absolutely fascinated by the way the visual world would appear when in motion. In the direction of the train, the visual world would appear to flow in the same direction and expand. When Gibson looked behind the train, the visual world would seem to contract. Later in life, Gibson would apply this fascination to the study of visual perception of landing and flying planes.

Related Articles